Archive for the ‘Temperaments’ Category
Entry 1737 — My Own Little Eurekaplex
Friday, February 27th, 2015
Today I derailed enthusiastically for a while into the following, which I now declare are the result of a Eurekaplex I myself constructed in my little brain:
A Maximally Simplistic Attempt to Describe the Workings of a Eurekaplex
To understand what a Eurekaplex is, one must first understand what a Rigidniplex is. That, in turn, requires one to understand what a Knowleplex is—so that’s where I’ll begin. At bottom, a knowleplex is just a complicated tangle of “molecules” of recorded knowledge (memories, in other words) that I call “knowlecules” arranged in a system of interconnecting “knowleculanes” that are stored in what is in effect an almost endlessly long container called “the Mnemoduct.” (Note: There are many mnemoducts, and thus many different knowleplexes, most of which occupy more than one mnemoduct. For the purposes of this essay, however, I will be considering only one of them. )
Knowlecules provide a person his understanding of small things, knowleculanes his knowledge of larger things, and knowleplexes his understanding of whole subject areas such as “The Author of the Works of Shakespeare” up to “The Science of Physics.” The easy way to think of these is to think of knowlecules as knowledge a single word can represent fairly fully whereas only a sentence or a paragraph can represent a knowleculane effectively while an essay or whole book, or library, is needed to come close to representing a knowleplex effectively.
Now, then, a rigidniplex is a knowleplex that, among many flaws, is excessively impervious to contradiction, irrational, and constricted. Despite all that, it may sometimes validly reflect reality, but most often it does not. The more rigidnikal a person is, the more his knowleplexes will be rigidniplexes.
(Editorial note: it was here that my too-potent accelerance mechanism took over.)
The major difference between normal knowleplex and a rigidniplex has to do with their responses to new data: the normal knowleplex is appreciably more hospitable to it, the rigidniplex sometimes near-impervious to it. It’s pretty simple, really. Let’s imagine a person’s knowleplex as a gated community only allowing entrance to external knowleculanes consisting of certain knowlecules in a certain order. Then imagine an external knowleculane comprised of the argument, “Meres referred to Buckhurst as a great writer of Tragedy but not as a great writer of comedies, or a writer at all of comedies; therefore, Buckhurst was NOT Shakespeare” shows up. If the person involved is normal (and knowledgeable about Elizabethan times), his knowleplex will run through its records of knowleculanes and find strands like “Meres referred to Buckhurst in 1598 . . .” It will allow the knowleculane entrance on the basis of its having a 4-element strand matching one or more of the strands the knowleplex found.
If, on the other hand, the person involved is a rigidnik (who believes Buckhurst was Shakespeare), his knowleplex will run through its records of knowleculanes looking for far longer matches, and refuse admittance as soon as it has found a strand representing, “Buckhurst was Shakespeare.”
* * *
My problem with this, I think, is that it’s too simplistic. I suddenly see much that needs amplification and I see no way to do it without killing its accessibility completely. I have to think about it. But the above is not completely worthless.
.
Entry 1568 — Me ‘n’ Riesman, Part 2
Friday, September 12th, 2014
After more reading of The Lonely Crowd, I’ve decided I’m very much inner-directed, according to Riesman’s description of the type. I got him wrong when I though his inner-directed type was similar to my rigidnik. I now an unsure how his autonomous type differs from his inner-directed type. According to Riesman, many of his readers, including colleagues of his, confused the two. I now see why–and Riesman himself seems to consider it a natural mistake. (He is excellently self-critical, it seems to me, but has surprising blind spots: for instance, about the possibility of innate psychological tendencies: he mentions such a possibility every once in a while, but quickly drops the subject, seeming to take social determinism the only important kind of determinism in the main body of his book–or so my impression is after not going very far in it.)
I’m also wondering how Riesman’s other-directed types ultimately differ from his tradition-directed types. Possibly, I just thought, because their memories coincide with their environmental input? They pray to whomever their tribal god is only partly because they’ve been trained to, but mostly because everyone else in the tribe is. The inner-directed person prays to his god because of his indoctrination entirely: he more or less has to because he is part of Riesman’s inner-directed society and thus not sure of having the right people to imitate.
The autonomous person will differ from the inner-directed person only in that he will be much more likely to question his indoctrination.
* * *
Last night while lying in bed hoping for sleep to come, I suddenly had a few ideas for poems, two of which follow:
intuition + reason = moonlight + pond
I’m not sure whether they’re finished or not, or whether, if finished, they’re keepers or not.
.
Entry 194 — The Knowleplex
Wednesday, August 18th, 2010
I’m still in my null zone but with enough zip to do the one thing I seem always ready and able to do: make up new Knowlecular Psychology terms. Not new is my term for any more or less interconnected body of knowledge, or inter-related group of knowlecules, the Knowleplex. The Knowlecule, in my theory, is the smallest datum, or bit of knowledge, in the context of whatever subject holds sway in a given mind: New York City, say, if the person is thinking about and/or discussing, the sociology of urban living (which would be a knowleplex); Broadway theatres if the knowleplex involved is The Culture of New York City.
Also not new is the term, Rigidniplex, for “irrational knowleplex formed and insanely or near-insanely adhered to by a rigidnik,” one of my temperament types. There are, so far, three other faulty knowleplexes in my system, each with a new name: the Indoctriplex, the Neurosiplex, and the Enthusiaplex. These are irrational fixation systems that act like rigidniplexes but have different causes.
The Rigidniplex comes about because of its owner’s charactration (mental energy) , which is too unalterably high for the flexibility required to recognize flaws and correct them. The Indoctriplex comes about because its owner’s charactration is too low for energy to revise flaws that the knowleplex contains due to intense, early indoctri- nation. It is the Milyoop’s equivalent of the Rigidnik’s knowleplex. The Neurosiplex can afflict anyone. It is an irrational knowleplex that comes about due to emotional trauma. A child who have never seen a dog, is nipped by one, and over-reacts, perhaps partly because the child’s mother over-reacts, and so much pain is attached to the event that the child develops a neurotic fear of dogs. “Neurosis” would be a good near-synonym for Neurosiplex. with Freud’s account of neuroses coming close to defining it, except for its neurophysiological basis.
Similar to the Neurosiplex but its etiological opposite is the Enthusiaplex. What forms its initial kernel is not emotional trauma but emotional ecstasy: the dog licks the child, the mother laughs, and the delighted child starts an irrational knowleplex concerning how wonderful dogs are. I found myself in need of such a knowleplex while trying to figure out how people who seemed reasonably sane could believe something as insane as the idea that Shakespeare did not write the works attributed to him. That they were rigidniks forced to believe as they did by their rigidniplexes explained the dominant anti-Strat- fordians, as they are called, as far as I was concerned, but there were others who were as nuts as the rigidnikal anti-Strat- fordians, but who showed few or no evidence in real life of being rigidniks, such as an insane reverence for formal education, a lack of aesthetic sensitivity, an incapacity for accepting anomalies in the historic record as due to anything other than some kind of official cover-up, etc.
I come to the conclusion that such people were freewenders who had come up with a seemingly rational counter-argument to the belief that Shakespeare was Shakespeare that was so enchantingly clever, and seemingly likely to be accepted by others, which would have all kinds of wonderful pay-offs (the way I felt about my theory of knowlecular psychology, in fact) that too much pleasure got attached to the initial insight for the freewenders ever thereafter to retract it.
The last of the knowleplexes my theory so far recognizes is the Verosoplex, which is a rational knowleplex (like all mine, needless to say); one, that is, which is based on fundaceptual data only (what our senses tell us) and the use of logic.
What does all this have to do with poetry? Well, I would say that the Poetry Establishment is dominated by people who have formed very narrow rigidniplexes about what poetry is. Ideas contrary to their set beliefs bounce off their rigidniplexes. Etc. Many of their milyoopian followers go along with them because of their indoctriplexes.
Certain freewenders develop idiosyncratic enthusiaplexes for poets who really aren’t very good, because they personally connect to their work–as someone from the working class might connect to Bukowski (actually, I like Bukowski, but not as loonily as his craziest fans), or a feminist to Anne Sexton.
The person who developed a neurotic fear of dogs might irrationally loathe any dog poem. Some, exposed to the crap some schools force on them, might form a neuroiplex against poetry.
The luckiest will form a verosoplex that allows them to at least tolerate almost any kind of poetry, and admire a wide range of poetry–more than the Wilshberian end of the poetry continuum.
Needless to say, all the above is a sketch. In real life, all is much more complex.